Last year, Nick Matzke of the ever informative evolutionary biology blog “The Panda’s Thumb” posted a neat little graph where he made a comparison of hominin brain sizes in relation to body sizes.

His post, “Fun with hominin brain size as a percentage of body mass” touches on one of my favorite topics in primatology, the evolution of primate brain evolution. One of the general characteristics that make primates unique are the larger brain sizes compared to body sizes in relation to other organism. His graph is limited in that it shows only the comparison of brain to body sizes of a tribe, within the family Hominidae, under a much larger taxonomic organization, the order Primates. This image (to your right) from the Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc. The increase in hominin cranial capacity over time.is a much illustrative but only depicts brain size evolution as a function of time.

This leads me to open a discussion on a fairly recent publication Evolutionary Anthropology, where Robert A. Barton, Director of the Evolutionary Anthropology Research Group at the University of Durham, U.K. addresses a much larger question,

“How did such variation evolve and why, and what are its cognitive implications?”

Robert Barton uses phylogenetic comparative methods to study the evolution of primate brains and behavior. His recent work has examined the evolution of neocortex size, the role of visual specialization in brain evolution, and ecological correlates of neural system evolution.

In the paper that I am talking about,”Primate brain evolution: Integrating comparative, neurophysiological, and ethological data,” Barton argues that people must be careful in using,

“comparative methods and in finding multiple converging strands of comparative evidence as opposed to making speculative interpretations of single correlations. In particular, recent work demonstrates the value of examining how evolutionary changes at different anatomical levels interrelate.”

What is of interest to primatologists out there, is that Barton tackles the adaptive significance of brain size in primates. He addresses a prominant assumption most people make about primates, that they are (and I’ve bolded what I consider important conclusions),

“distinguished from most other mammals by superior cognitive abilities. Large relative brain size is the most obvious justification for this view. Why did large brains evolve and what is it that they enable primates to do?

One way to tackle these questions is to examine which brain systems, with what functional properties, are associated with increased brain size. As noted, the neocortex is disproportionately expanded in primates compared with at least some other mammals, and anthropoid primates have larger neocortices than do lemurs and lorises. The fact that within primates variation in neocortex size relative to the rest of the brain is highly correlated with encephalization emphasizes the role of “neocorticalization” in primate brain expansion.

Relative expansion of the neocortex, however, reveals comparatively little about which functional systems are involved, and hence what is the adaptive significance of overall brain size. As described, the functional systems of the brain cut across the major subdivisions. No known cognitive process is mediated exclusively by the neocortex. Complex cognitive processes are mediated by networks that link the neocortex with many other structures. Furthermore, the neocortex is a highly heterogenous organ, processing information from all the senses and being involved in many different aspects of sensory, motor, and cognitive processing. Hence, the neocortex is necessary for many cognitive functions but sufficient for none. It is therefore misleading to view the neocortex as the “cognitive” part of the brain. As with overall brain size, we still need to identify the specific systems in which selective expansion accounts for the increase in overall neocortex size and hence, in brain size.

Barton goes on to show how other fields have made premature conclusions. Since we know so little about the function of the brain, he wraps up his paper with an argument that we need a greater understanding of brain evolution, one which

“depends on studying and interrelating evolutionary change at a variety of levels, from microscopic to macroscopic anatomy and from neural systems to behavioral ecology. While it has been fashionable in some circles to denigrate brain size as a crude measure of cognitive abilities, to an evolutionary biologist the manifest variation in brain size is intrinsically interesting and demanding of an explanation. Yet, in order to explain variation in brain size we need to understand which systems, with what functional properties, contribute to it, and what constraints act on it. Conversely, cognitive neuroscience may learn valuable lessons from phylogenetic comparative studies.”

I completely side with this advice Barton has given, however I don’t know how that will stop or prevent people from making conclusions in general. Many evolutionary biologists like Lewontin, Gould, etc. have argued (in the 70′s!) that we should not make ‘adaptionist’ stories to justify why we see certain adaptations… but it is still prevalent to this day.

Also, let me personally interject on Barton’s advice by adding that in order to understand primate brain evolution we also need to decipher a larger understanding of comparative and functional genomics between primates. We need to understand what genes are facilitating the development of primate brains, and how they have changed throughout evolutionary time.

About these ads